Term
|
Definition
Refers to the rules governing the structure and sequence of speech sound. the ability to of analyzing so fast the languange in you minds as words.
|
|
|
Term
|
Definition
The second component, involves vocabulary-the way underlying concepts are expressed in words and combinations. preschoolers have to refine the meaning of the words and connect them into elaborated networks of related terms.
|
|
|
Term
|
Definition
Consists on two main parts: syntax, the roles by which words are arranged into sentences, and Morphology, the use of grammatical markers indicating numbers, tense, case, person, gender , active or passive voice, and other meanings (the ending -s and -ed are examples in English)
|
|
|
Term
|
Definition
Refers to the rule for engaging in appropriate and effective communication. (taking turns, stay on the same topic, figure out gestures, tone of voice. They must adjust their speech to mark important social relationships, such as difference between age and gender, and they have to master their culture narrative so they can communicate with their people.
|
|
|
Term
Language Acquisition Device
|
|
Definition
an innate module for picking up language which is triggered by
verbal input
|
|
|
Term
|
Definition
touching, holding, or pointing at an object as a means of directing other’s attention
|
|
|
Term
|
Definition
getting somebody to do something by reaching, pointing, and making sounds (EX Christian’s grunting and breathing excitedly)
|
|
|
Term
|
Definition
apply words too narrowly; inability to see a word belongs to many things instead of just one (EX the family cat is the only “cat” there is)
|
|
|
Term
|
Definition
applying a word to a broader collection of objects and events than appropriate (EX calling a dog “cat” because it has four legs like a cat; a toddler might uses “car” to describe buses, trains, and trucks)
|
|
|
Term
|
Definition
once children apply a regular morphological rule, they extend it to words that are exceptions (EX “my toy car breaked”; “we each have two foots”
|
|
|
Term
|
Definition
Children can connect a new word with an underlying concept after only a brief encounter. Ex. An adult picks up a fruit and says she wants to eat an orange. The child may understand that an orange is what the adult has in her hand
|
|
|
Term
|
Definition
A hypothesis that says preschoolers discover word meanings by observing how words are used in syntax, or the structure of sentences. Ex: An adult shows a child a green ball and says “This is bright” The child will conclude that bright has something to do with the green ball
|
|
|
Term
|
Definition
Use word meanings to figure out sentence structure. Children might begin by grouping together words with “agent qualities” (things that cause actions) as subjects and words with “action qualities” as verbs. Then they merge these categories with observations of how words are used in sentences.
|
|
|
Term
|
Definition
Two word utterances. They focus on high-content words and omit smaller, less important ones. Ex. Instead of saying “I want some milk” Children say “want milk”
|
|
|
Term
|
Definition
The speaker not only comments on what has just been said but also adds a request to get the partner to respond again. 2-year-olds cannot generate turnabouts, but are able to as they increase in age. Ex. Conversation skills. Person 1: Can I have a cookie? Person 2: Yes you may. Person 1: Did you have a cookie? Person 2: No I didn’t, are they good?
|
|
|
Term
|
Definition
Restructuring inaccurate speech into correct form. Ex. Child: “Mommy goed to the store” Adult: “Yes, your mom went to the store.”
|
|
|
Term
|
Definition
Elaborating on children’s speech, increasing in complexity. Ex. Child: “My shirt is pink” Adult: “Yes, your new shirt is pink and has flowers on it.”
|
|
|
Term
|
Definition
A speaker initiates a change of topic gradually by modifying the focus of discussion. Appears between ages 5-9. Child: “What’s for dinner” Adult: “We’re having pizza” Child: “I love pizza! That reminds me, when are we having my party.”
|
|
|
Term
|
Definition
the ability to think about language as a system. to know what the words mean. A 4 year old thinks that typewriter and typewrite is the same, when they get to 5 they can recognize all the words, and it is reached in middle childhood, 4-8 years.
|
|
|
Term
|
Definition
what a speaker means to say, even if the form of the utterance is not perfectly consistent with it. Comprehend a variety of requests for actions not directly expressed as requests. “I need a pencil” “why don’t you tickle me?”
|
|
|
Term
|
Definition
producing clear verbal message and must recognize when a message we receive are unclear so we can ask for more information to communicate effectivly.
|
|
|
Term
Motherese (Child Direct Speech)
|
|
Definition
short sentences with high-pitched, exaggerated expressions, clear, pronunciation, and distinct pauses between speech segments
|
|
|
Term
|
Definition
Gazing in the same direction as adults (so they can see what a parent is verbally referring to.)
|
|
|