Term
|
Definition
The system for combining words into sentences |
|
|
Term
Phonologically less stressed Semantically less important to Cognitive limitations (concepts conveyed are more abstract) Harder to define ostensively (e.g., can’t “point to” an ING or THE) |
|
Definition
Why do children preferentially leave out grammatical morphemes? |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
Allows for new members (can make new ones)nouns, verbs, and adjectives |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
Does not allow for new members articles, auxiliary verbs, prepositions |
|
|
Term
Brown’s 14 Grammatical Morphemes |
|
Definition
Present Progressive (-ing) In On Plural (-s) Past Irregular (e.g., came, went) Possessive (-‘s) Uncontractible Copula (am, is, are, was, were) Articles (a, the) Past Regular (-d) Third-Person Regular (-s; e.g., she talks) Third-Person Irregular (e.g., does, has) Uncontractible Auxiliary (am, is, are, has, have) Contractible Copula (‘m, ‘s, ‘re) Contractible Auxiliary (‘m, ‘s, ‘re when combined with –ing; ‘ve, ‘s when combined with a past participle such as has been) |
|
|
Term
Mean Length of Utterance (MLU) |
|
Definition
A common measure of grammatical development. It is the average length of the utterances in a sample of spontaneous speech, usually counted in terms of the number of morphemes |
|
|
Term
Stage 1 (MLU = 1 to 2). Children combine words, but essentially no grammatical morphemes Stage 2 (MLU = 2 to 2.5). Grammatical morphemes start coming in Stage 3 (MLU = 2.5 – 3). Children begin forming non-declarative sentences (questions, negatives) Stage 4 (MLU = 3-4) More complex sentence types added Stage 5 (MLU = 4 to 4.5). Embedded sentences |
|
Definition
Correspondence between MLU & children’s utterances, 5 stages |
|
|
Term
Acquisition of syntactic categories |
|
Definition
a. Semantic categories = (agent, object, action) b. Syntactic categories = (N, V, etc) |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
Children begin to figure out grammar by assigning grammatical structure to sentences based on their meaning. ii. Words for people, places, and things as Nouns |
|
|
Term
Prosodic bootstrapping (phonological bootstrapping) |
|
Definition
i. Children use phonological & prosodic cues to break into grammatical structure ii. Nouns tend to have first-syllable stress, while verbs have second-syllable stress 1. record (say it as a noun and a verb..note the difference) |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
a. Noun, Verb, Adjective, Preposition, Determiner(article), Sentence (aka complementizer phrased) |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
a. A series of actions in which the D-structure is transformed into the s-structure |
|
|
Term
syntactically based grammar |
|
Definition
A grammar in which rules operate over formal categories, such as Noun and Verb. These formal categories are not defined in terms of their meaning or their communicative function. |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
|
|
Term
SAI (Subject-auxiliary inversion) |
|
Definition
i. Move the first aux element to the sentence initial position 1. John will see Bill tomorrow. 2. Will John see Bill tomorrow? |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
Children have syntactic categories from the beginning, they just tend to produce nouns that are agents and object etc. What children say (their performance) is only a tiny fraction of what they know (competence). |
|
|
Term
Continuity acquisitional theories (Iceberg) |
|
Definition
Children's grammars are fundamentally the same as adults |
|
|
Term
21. Discontinuity acquisitional theories (tadpole) |
|
Definition
a. Children's grammar mature or unfold over time based on a genetically pre-specified, linguistically-specific biological program i. How tadpoles become frogs! |
|
|
Term
Maturational theories of language acquisition |
|
Definition
Syntactic categories appear later as the result of biological maturation i. Early speech displays a semantic to syntactic system as a result of maturational processes. |
|
|
Term
Optimality Theory & Syntax Acquisition |
|
Definition
o Languages share the same constraints Languages differ in the ranking of the constraints |
|
|