Term
|
Definition
Bizarre material is better remembered than common material. |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
The tendency to remember one's choices as better than they actually were.[121] |
|
|
Term
Conservatism or Regressive bias |
|
Definition
Tendency to remember high values and high likelihoods/probabilities/frequencies as lower than they actually were and low ones as higher than they actually were. Based on the evidence, memories are not extreme enough.[122][123] |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
Incorrectly remembering one's past attitudes and behaviour as resembling present attitudes and behaviour.[124] |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
That cognition and memory are dependent on context, such that out-of-context memories are more difficult to retrieve than in-context memories (e.g., recall time and accuracy for a work-related memory will be lower at home, and vice versa). |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
The tendency for people of one race to have difficulty identifying members of a race other than their own. |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
A form of misattribution where a memory is mistaken for imagination, because there is no subjective experience of it being a memory.[125] |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
Recalling the past in a self-serving manner, e.g., remembering one's exam grades as being better than they were, or remembering a caught fish as bigger than it really was. |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
A bias in which the emotion associated with unpleasant memories fades more quickly than the emotion associated with positive events.[126] |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
A form of misattribution where imagination is mistaken for a memory. |
|
|
Term
Generation effect (Self-generation effect) |
|
Definition
That self-generated information is remembered best. For instance, people are better able to recall memories of statements that they have generated than similar statements generated by others. |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
The tendency to forget information that can be found readily online by using Internet search engines. |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
That humorous items are more easily remembered than non-humorous ones, which might be explained by the distinctiveness of humor, the increased cognitive processing time to understand the humor, or the emotional arousal caused by the humor.[127] |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
The phenomenon whereby learning is greater when studying is spread out over time, as opposed to studying the same amount of time in a single session. See also spacing effect. |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
Memory distortions introduced by the loss of details in a recollection over time, often concurrent with sharpening or selective recollection of certain details that take on exaggerated significance in relation to the details or aspects of the experience lost through leveling. Both biases may be reinforced over time, and by repeated recollection or re-telling of a memory.[128] |
|
|
Term
Levels-of-processing effect |
|
Definition
That different methods of encoding information into memory have different levels of effectiveness.[129] |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
A smaller percentage of items are remembered in a longer list, but as the length of the list increases, the absolute number of items remembered increases as well. For example, consider a list of 30 items ("L30") and a list of 100 items ("L100"). An individual may remember 15 items from L30, or 50%, whereas the individual may remember 40 items from L100, or 40%. Although the percent of L30 items remembered (50%) is greater than the percent of L100 (40%), more L100 items (40) are remembered than L30 items (15).[130][further explanation needed] |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
Memory becoming less accurate because of interference from post-event information.[131] |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
That memory recall is higher for the last items of a list when the list items were received via speech than when they were received through writing. |
|
|
Term
Mood-congruent memory bias |
|
Definition
The improved recall of information congruent with one's current mood. |
|
|
Term
Negativity bias or Negativity effect |
|
Definition
Psychological phenomenon by which humans have a greater recall of unpleasant memories compared with positive memories.[132][87] (see also actor-observer bias, group attribution error, positivity effect, and negativity effect).[108] |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
When taking turns speaking in a group using a predetermined order (e.g. going clockwise around a room, taking numbers, etc.) people tend to have diminished recall for the words of the person who spoke immediately before them.[133] |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
That being shown some items from a list and later retrieving one item causes it to become harder to retrieve the other items.[134] |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
That people seem to perceive not the sum of an experience but the average of how it was at its peak (e.g., pleasant or unpleasant) and how it ended. |
|
|
Term
Picture superiority effect |
|
Definition
The notion that concepts that are learned by viewing pictures are more easily and frequently recalled than are concepts that are learned by viewing their written word form counterparts.[135][136][137][138][139][140] |
|
|
Term
Positivity effect (Socioemotional selectivity theory) |
|
Definition
That older adults favor positive over negative information in their memories. |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
That items near the end of a sequence are the easiest to recall, followed by the items at the beginning of a sequence; items in the middle are the least likely to be remembered.[141] |
|
|
Term
Processing difficulty effect |
|
Definition
That information that takes longer to read and is thought about more (processed with more difficulty) is more easily remembered.[142] |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
The recalling of more personal events from adolescence and early adulthood than personal events from other lifetime periods.[143] |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
That memories relating to the self are better recalled than similar information relating to others. |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
Confusing episodic memories with other information, creating distorted memories.[144] |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
That information is better recalled if exposure to it is repeated over a long span of time rather than a short one. |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
The tendency to overestimate the amount that other people notice your appearance or behavior. |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
Memory distorted towards stereotypes (e.g., racial or gender). |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
Diminishment of the recency effect because a sound item is appended to the list that the subject is not required to recall.[145][146] |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
A form of misattribution where ideas suggested by a questioner are mistaken for memory. |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
When time perceived by the individual either lengthens, making events appear to slow down, or contracts.[147] |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
The tendency to displace recent events backwards in time and remote events forward in time, so that recent events appear more remote, and remote events, more recent. |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
The fact that you more easily remember information you have read by rewriting it instead of rereading it.[148] |
|
|
Term
Tip of the tongue phenomenon |
|
Definition
When a subject is able to recall parts of an item, or related information, but is frustratingly unable to recall the whole item. This is thought to be an instance of "blocking" where multiple similar memories are being recalled and interfere with each other.[125] |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
Overestimating the significance of the present.[149] It is related to chronological snobbery with possibly an appeal to novelty logical fallacy being part of the bias. |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
That the "gist" of what someone has said is better remembered than the verbatim wording.[150] This is because memories are representations, not exact copies. |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
That an item that sticks out is more likely to be remembered than other items.[151] |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
That uncompleted or interrupted tasks are remembered better than completed ones. |
|
|