Term
|
Definition
The process by which simulation of a sensory receptor produces neural impulses that the brain interprets as a sound, a visual image, an odor, a taste, a pain, or other sensory image. Sensation represents the first series of steps in processing of incoming information. |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
A process that makes sensory patterns meaningful. It is perception that makes these words meaningful, rather than just a string of visual patterns. To make this happen, perception draws heavily on memory, motivation, emotion, and other psychological processes. |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
Transformation of one form of energy into another – especially the transformation of stimulus information into nerve signals by the sense organs. Without transduction, ripe tomatoes would not appear red. |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
Loss of responsiveness in receptor cells after stimulation has remained unchanged for a while, as when a swimmer becomes adapted to the temperature of the water. |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
The amount of stimulation necessary for a stimulus to be detected. In practice, this means that the presence or absence of a stimulus is detected correctly half the time over many trials. |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
The smallest amount by which a stimulus can be changed and the difference be detected half the time. |
|
|
Term
Just noticeable difference (JND) |
|
Definition
Same as the difference threshold. |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
This concept says that the size of a JND is proportional to the intensity of the stimulus; the JND is large when the stimulus intensity is high and is small when the stimulus intensity is low. |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
The magnitude of a stimulus can be estimated by the formula S=klogR, where S = sensation, R = stimulus, and k = a constant that differs for every sensory modality (sight, touch, temperature, etc) |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
A law of magnitude estimation that is more accurate than Fechner's law and covers a wider variety of stimuli. It is represented by the formula S=kl^a, where S = sensation, K= a constant, l = stimulus intensity, and a = a power exponent that depends on the sense being measured. |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
Explains how he detect "signals" consisting of stimulation affectiing our eyes, ears, nose, skin, and other sense organs. Signal detection theory says that sensation is a judgment the sensory system makes about incoming stimulation. Often, it occurs outside of consciousness. In contrast to older theories from psychophysics, signal detection theory takes observer characteristics into account. |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
The thin, light-sensitive layer at the back of the eyeball. The retina contains millions of photoreceptors and other nerve cells. |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
Light-sensitive cells(neurons) in the retina that convert light energy to neural impulses. The photoreceptors are as far as light gets into the visual system. |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
Photoreceptors in the retina that are especially sensitive to dim light but not to colors. Strange as it may seem, they are rod-shaped. |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
Photoreceptors in the retina that are especially sensitive to colors but not to dim light. They are cone-shaped. |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
The tiny area of sharpest vision in the retina. |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
The bundle of neurons that carries visual information from the retina to the brain. |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
The point where the optic nerve esits the eye and where there are no photoreceptors. Any stimulus that falls on this area can't be seen. |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
A psychological sensation caused by intensity of light waves. |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
Also called hue. Color is not a property of things in the external world. Rather, it is a psychological sensation created in the brain from information obtained by the eyes from the wavelengths of visible light. |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
The entire range of electromagnetic energy, including radio waves, X rays, microwaves, and visible light. |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
The tiny part of the electromagnetic spectrum to which our eyes are sensitive. The visible spectrum of other creatures may be slightly different from our own. |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
The idea that colors are sensed by three different types of cones sensitive to light in the red, blue, and green wavelengths. The trichromatic theory explains the earliest stage of color sensation. |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
The idea that cells in the visual system process colors in complementary pairs, such as red or green or as yellow or blue. The opponent-process theory explains color sensation from the bipolar cells onward in the visual system. |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
Sensations that linger after the stimulus is removed. Most visual afterimages are negative afterimages, which appear in reversed colors. |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
Typically a genetic disorder (although sometimes the result of trauma) that prevents an individual from discriminating certain colors. The most common form is red-green color blindness. |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
The number of cycles completed by a wave in a given amount of time, usually a second. |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
The physical strength of a wave. This is usually measured from peak to valley on a graph of the wave. |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
|
|
Term
|
Definition
The primary organ of hearing; a coiled tube in the inner ear where sound waves are transduced into nerve messages. |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
A thin strip of tissue sensitive to vibrations in the cochlea. The basilar membrane contains hair cells connected to neurons. When a sound wave causes the hair cells to vibrate, the associated neurons become excited. As a result, the sound waves are converted(transduced) into nerve activity. |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
A sensory characteristic of sound produced by the frequency of the sound wave. |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
A sensory characteristic of sound produced by the amplitude (intensity) of the sound wave. |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
The quality of a sound wave that derives from the wave's complexity (combination of pure tones). Timbre comes from the Greek word for 'drum'. |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
An inability to hear resulting from damage to structures of the middle or inner ear. |
|
|
Term
Nerve deafness (Sensorineural deafness) |
|
Definition
An inability to hear linked to a deficit in the body's ability to transmit impulses from the cochlea to the brain, usually involving the auditory nerve or higher auditory processing centers. |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
The sense of the body orientation with respect to gravity. The vestibular sense is closely associated with the inner ear and, in fact, is carried to the brain on a branch of the auditory nerve. |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
The sense of body position and movement of body parts relative to each other. |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
|
|
Term
|
Definition
Chemical signals released by organisms to communicate with other members of their species. Pheromones are often used by animals as sexual attractants. It is unclear whether or not humans employ pheromones. |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
The sense of taste - from the same word root as 'gusto' - also called the gustatory sense. |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
Sensory systems for processing touch, warmth, cold, texture, and pain. |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
An explanation for pain control that proposes we have a neural 'gate' that can, under some circumstances, block incoming pain signals. |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
A response to a placebo (a fake drug) caused by subjects' belief that they are taking real drugs. |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
The meaningful product of perception - often an image that has been associated with concepts, memories of events, emotions, and motives. |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
Cells in the cortex that specialize in extracting certain features of a stimulus. |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
Refers to the process used by the brain to combine (or bind) the results of many sensory operations into a single percept. This occurs, for example, when sensations of color, shape, boundary, and texture are combined to produce the percept of a person's face. No one knows exactly how the brain does this. Thus the binding problem is one of the major unsolved mysteries in psychology. |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
Perceptual analysis that emphasizes characteristics of the stimulus, rather than our concepts and expectations. Bottom refers to the syimulus, which occurs at step one of perceptual processing. |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
Perceptual analysis that emphasizes the perceiver's expectations, concept memories, and other cognitive factors, rahter than being driven by the characteristics of the stimulus. Top refers to a mental state in the brain - which stands at the 'top' of the perceptual processing system. |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
The ability to recognize the same object as remaining 'constant' under different conditions, such as changes in illumination, distance, or location. |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
You have experienced an illusion when you have a demonstrably incorrect perception of a stimulus pattern, especially one that also fools others who are observing the same stimulus. |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
Images that are capable of more than one interpretation. There is no 'right' way to see an ambiguous figure. |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
From a German word that means 'whole' or 'form' or 'configuration' (A Gestalt is also a percept). The Gestalt psychologists believed that much of perception is shaped by innate factors built into the brain. |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
The part of a pattern that commands attention. The figure stands out against the ground. |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
The part of a pattern that does not command attention; the background. |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
The Gestalt principle that identifies the tendency to fill in gaps in figures and to see incomplete figures as complete. |
|
|
Term
Laws or perceptual grouping |
|
Definition
The Gestalt principles of similarity, proximity, continuity, and common fate. These 'laws' suggest how are brains prefer to group stimulus elements together to form a percept. |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
The Gestalt principle that we tend to group similar objects together in our perceptions. |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
The Gestalt principle that we tend to group objects together when they are near each other. |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
The Gestalt principle that we prefer perceptions of connected and continuous figures to disconnected and disjointed ones. |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
The Gestalt principle that we tend to group similar objects together that share a common motion or destination. |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
The most general Gestalt principle, which states that the simplest organization, requiring the least cognitive effort, will emerge as the figure. Pragnanz shares a common root with pregnant, and so it carries the idea of a 'fully developed figure'. That is, our perceptual system prefers to see a fully developed Gestalt, such as the complete circle, as opposed to a broken circle. |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
Information taken in by both eyes that aids in depth perception, including binocular convergence and retinal disparity. |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
Information about depth that relies on the input of just one eye - includes relative size, light and shadow, interposition, relative motion, and atmospheric perspective. |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
The view that perception is primarily shaped by learning (or experience), rather than by innate factors. |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
Readiness to detect a particular stimulus in a given context - as when a person who is afraid interprets an unfamiliar sound in the night as a threat. |
|
|