Term
Piaget's Stages of Cognitive Development |
|
Definition
Sensorimotor, Preoperational, Concrete operations, Formal operations |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
first stage of Piaget's cognitive development theory; age 0-2 obtain information through senses and motor skills attains object permanence, ability to form mental representations, imaginative play |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
second stage of Piaget's cognitive development theory; age 2-6 interact with world by how things appear development of symbolic function, beginnings of attainment of conservation, limited by egocentrism, animism, irreversibility, and centration |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
giving human characteristics to inanimate objects |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
cannot think about a process forward and backward |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
have trouble focusing on more than one dimension |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
third stage of Piaget's cognitive development theory; age 6 or 7-11 or 12 stop relying on appearance and start using logical thought onset of logical thinking, conservation, decentration, reversibility, ability to see others' points of view, classification, seriation |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
fourth stage of Piaget's cognitive development theory; age 11 or 12 ability to think in the abstract, flexibility, mental hypothesis testing, ability to consider alternatives in problem solving |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
child will look for hidden object under the first place it was hidden |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
constructivist: children have an active role in constructing their cognition children try to make sense of the world based on previous knowledge and experience; use to process new information 50/50 nature/nurture |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
mental representations of a thing, place, person, etc. |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
applying existing schemas to new information |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
children can learn tasks that do not have anything to do with problem solving development comes from language 80/20 nurture/nature |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
the role that the adult plays in offering a new concept or term |
|
|
Term
Zone of proximal development |
|
Definition
bottom level: what the child can do on their own within zone: stages where child may need assistance top level: tasks the child cannot do at all |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
the assistance that is provided by the elder; should be gradually reduced as the child acquires the skill |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
children working together with their peers and both children arrive at the solution |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
the child starts as the observer because they are not capable of doing the task yet, then the adult walks them through the steps of the task, and then the child practices each step individually with the adult (ex: weaving a blanket- child watches, then works the pedals, then starts weaving, etc.) |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
children saying all their private thoughts out loud; most often during novel tasks; Piaget saw no value in this; disappears around age 10, peaks at age 4-6 Vygotsky— value = direct own behavior, acquire new skills, work through situations that are unfamiliar; thought that at age 10 it becomes internalized speech |
|
|
Term
Information-Processing Model |
|
Definition
Atkinson and Shiffrin, 1968 adult humans can only hold sensory information in the sensory registry for 250 milliseconds; sensory register narrows down what you focus on |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
age 0-4: spend only a short time involved in a task, can't focus on details, and are easily distracted starts to improve around age 2-3.5 3-4: cognitive inhibition 6-10: better at selective attention- focus on aspects of a situation that are relevant to their goal 8-10: develop attentional strategies |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
age 3-4; able to ignore distractions |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
an experience that is highly emotional automatically transfers to long-term store |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
takes effort to get the material into long-term store for children involves mnemonic strategies, rehearsal |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
generate a list of items and ask the person to recall them in any order little ones use passive rehearsal |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
children rehearse one thing one time; ball ball, horse horse; not very effective |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
used by older kids and adults; multi-item rehearsal if you teach small children to memorize multiple items, they will have better memory |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
used by Greek speakers; imagine a street and associated points in the speech with a visual image of walking down the street |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
another word for schemata; starts out as primitive and grows and becomes rich with age |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
gave college students that had no chess experience and third grade chess experts digits to remember, saying that they were chess positions; the children remembered more due to knowledge base |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
Case; whenever you're engaging in a task where you have to process info, you only have a certain amount of attentional capacity to devote to the task and to remembering; adults tend to not use as much AC on the actual strategy, and use more AC on remembering (vice versa for children) |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
overarching idea of knowing how your mind works, that your thoughts are different from everyone else's |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
a subset of metacognitive skills; refers to a child's knowledge of how their memory works; gets better over time |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
meanings associated with a word; takes a long time for children to get |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
meanings associated with a word; takes a long time for children to get |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
|
|
Term
|
Definition
smallest units of sound (single letters) |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
infants put phonemes together in ways that match the language they are surrounded by |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
the beginnings of words; "puh" for "pink blanket" |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
12 months; saying one word to mean a phrase; "cookie" means "I want that cookie", or "bottle" mean "the bottle is over there" |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
correct order of words in a sentence; knowing how to form plurals and past tense, etc. |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
18-24 months; only use the most critical words; "want cookie" or "no bath"; still putting nouns and verbs together properly |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
2.5-5 years; forming more grammatically correct phrases "where is kitty" |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
3 years; putting words in right order, using adjectives as adjectives etc. |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
forming incorrect plurals or past tense with irregular verbs and nouns; "I runned away" or "I saw deers" |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
18 months; children pick up the gist of a word even after singular exposure to it; learn about 10 words per day until high school graduation |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
use assumptions based on context to come up with meanings for words |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
overapplying a newly learned vocabulary word; an ostrich applies to every animal in a cage |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
underapplying a newly learned vocabulary word; the family's pet parrot is the only parrot anywhere, so parrots at the zoo or on TV are not parrots |
|
|
Term
Syntactical bootstrapping |
|
Definition
using the syntax in the sentence to help figure out the meaning of a word |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
the etiquette of language; the proper social rules of discourse; turn-taking, question and answer |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
when you ask a rhetorical question, the child starts to understand it does not require an answer; "do you have to jump on your bed?" "how many times have I told you to put your shoes away?" |
|
|
Term
Referential communication |
|
Definition
modifying language in order to be understood |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
we see different stages of language development at the same ages across cultures |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
happens when children are content and alone; hypothesized that this is reinforcing, that they like to hear themselves |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
Berko; proves child's extraction of grammar rules make up words that don't exist, but children can still apply grammar "This is a wug. Now here comes another wug. Now there are...two wugs." |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
used children's universal errors in language to show that they are primed to learn language through grammar vs. just learning their parents' language language acquisition device |
|
|
Term
Language acquisition device (LAD) |
|
Definition
children are primed to pick up on human voices and languages; born with innate predisposition to acquire language; universal pattern of language development |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
creation of a language from two previous languages so that people of different areas can communicate |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
parents scaffold language to accommodate their children's level of speech |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
Social referencing; conducted experiments with babies learning to crawl; mother's facial expression convinced them to cross a short visual cliff |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
put rouge on a 16-month-old boy's nose, and he thought his reflection in the mirror was his sister; did same test on an 18-month-old, and he looks embarrassed sense of self develops around 18 months |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
studied cautious children vs outgoing children; some are just born with a predisposition for one or the other |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
wanted to isolate the features of the face that would be associated with each emotion— children universally respond with same expressions, suggesting it is biological |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
infants crawling for more than a month would not cross the deep visual cliff, even with their parents smiling at them |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
child looks to mother in difficult situations |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
a dominant way of responding to the environment |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
ongoing study in child development 1956- NY Longitudinal Study 141 children participated- wanted to assess and measure temperament developed nine categories of temperamental characteristics (page ); gave the list to parents and asked them to report on their child's temperament (activity level, rhythmicity, approach or withdrawal, adaptability, threshold of responsiveness, intensity of reaction, quality of mood, distractibility, attention span and persistence) in their sample, approximately 40% had an "easy temperament" (moderate degree of activity, regular eating and sleeping, normal caution, good adaptability); 10% had a "difficult temperament" (had extreme reactions to novel stimuli, hard time adjusting to novel situations, did not eat and sleep regularly); 15% were "slow to warm up" (cautious at first, slow to respond; but with repeated exposure, eventually warmed up) Developmental outcomes will be best for the child if the parenting style matches the temperament |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
emotional tie to a caregiver Bowlby |
|
|
Term
|
Definition
page 303- observed children in post-WWII Europe; found that orphaned children were sad and sick, despite normal caregiving; did not have attachment to caregiver Four stages of attachment -First few months: attached to no one -2-6 months: begin to discriminate between caregivers (usually mother) -6 months-3 years: 6-12 month (specific attachment to caregiver; separation anxiety and fear of strangers peaks); -3 years and up: attachment to daycare workers, etc. |
|
|